front 1
Explain the difference between open and forced-choice formats
for survey questions. | back 1 - open questions: allow answers to be answered any way they
want
- drawback= responses must be coded and
categorized
- Forced-choice formats: people give their
opinion by picking the best of 2 or more choices
- used
in political polls, measure personality, yes/no format
|
| back 2 - allow answers to be answered any way they want
- drawback= responses must be coded and categorized
|
| back 3 - people give their opinion by picking the best of 2 or more
choices
- used in political polls, measure personality,
yes/no format
|
front 4
Define the three ethical principles from the Belmont report
and how each is applied. | back 4 - Principle of Respect for Persons
- Principle of
Beneficence
- Principle of Justice
|
front 5
Principle of Respect for Persons | back 5 - Individuals have autonomy and should be free to make up their
own minds if they would like to participate or not
- Applied
→ Every person is entitled to the precaution of informed consent
(the right of research participants to learn about a research
project, know its risks and benefits, and decide whether to
participate).
|
| back 6 - access potential harm to participants & potential benefits
to society
- do not collect identifying information
- applied → make participants information either anonymously or
confidential
- researchers must take precautions to
protect participants from harm and ensure their well-being
- applied → researchers must carefully assess the risks and
benefits of the study they plan to conduct
- how the
community might benefit or be harmed
|
| back 7 - Who bears the burden of research participation
- fair
balance between the kinds of people who participate in research and
the kinds of people that benefits from the research
- applied
→ researchers consider the extent to which the participants involved
in a study are representative of the kinds of people who would also
benefit from the results
|
front 8
Define the 5 APA ethical principles and how each is applied. | back 8 - A. Beneficence and nonmaleficence
- B. Fidelity &
responsibility
- C. Integrity
- D. Justice
- E.
Respect for rights & dignity
|
front 9
A. Beneficence and nonmaleficence | back 9 - treat people in ways that benefit them, do not cause suffering.
conduct reserach that will benefit society
|
front 10
B. Fidelity & responsibility | back 10 - establish relationships of trust; accept responsibility for
professional behavior (in research, teaching and clinical
practice)
|
| back 11 - strive to be accurate, truthful and honest in one’s role as
researcher, teacher or practitioner
|
| back 12 - strive to treat all groups of people fairly
- sample
research participants from the same populations that will benefit
from the research
- be aware of biases
|
front 13
E. Respect for rights & dignity | back 13 - Recognize that people are autonomous agents. Protect people’s
rights, including the right to privacy, the right to give consent
for treatment or research, and the right to have participation
treated confidentially.
- Understand that some populations
may be less able to give autonomous consent, and take precautions
against coercing such people.
|
front 14
What are the three major ethics violations from the Tuskegee
Syphilis Study? | back 14 -
The participants
were not treated respectfully
- Researchers
lied to them about the nature of their participation and withheld
information
- With this, they didn’t give the men a chance to
make a fly informed decision about participating in the study
-
The participants
were harmed
- participants were subject to
painful and dangerous tests
- participants and their
families were not told of the treatment until years later, this
could have cured them
-
The researchers
targeted a disadvantaged social group
- syphilis affects all people from all social backgrounds and
ethnicities, yet all the men from the stuy are poor and African
American
|
front 15
What are some of the ethical questions associated with the
Milgram study? | back 15 - To what extent was it ethical to put unsuspecting volunteers
through such a stressful experince?
- if ethical → trying to
balance the potential risks to participants and value of knowlage
gained
|
front 16
What does it mean to say we are balancing risks to
participants with risks to society? What other needs are balanced
when conducting an ethical study with human participants? | back 16 - means we are balancing potential risks and knowledge
gained
- ,. when a study puts the participants in a
harmful or stressful situation, how do the benefits weigh out →
Milgrim's study was good in outweighing the risks.
- benefits to society to risks and coercion or rewards to the
involved participation, debriefing of the study to the participants,
reference to the 5 ethical principles
|
front 17
Explain the problems with poorly worded survey questions, such
as leading, double-barreled, or negatively worded questions. Give
examples of each. | back 17 - question-wording matters because you need to know how to ask
questions without biasing participants against each other
- Different versions of the question can lead to different
results
- leading: can be problematic b/c wording
encourages one response more than others
- weakens
construct validity
- doubled-barreled
- asks 2 questions in 1
- weakens construct validity
b/c participants would answer either the 1st or 2nd half of
question
- may have different answers
- ex. Do
you enjoy swimming & wearing sunscreen?
- negatively worded questions:
- question is
incomprehensible and confusing
- contains negatively
phrased statements and weakens construct validity
- ex.
“People who do not drive with a suspended license should never be
punished”
- Question order (?)
|
| back 18 - can be problematic b/c wording encourages one response more
than others
- weakens construct validity
|
| back 19 - asks 2 questions in 1
- weakens construct validity
b/c participants would answer either the 1st or 2nd half of
question
- may have different answers
- ex. Do
you enjoy swimming & wearing sunscreen?
|
front 20
negatively worded questions | back 20 - question is incomprehensible and confusing
- contains
negatively phrased statements and weakens construct validity
- ex. “People who do not drive with a suspended license should
never be punished”
|
front 21
What are some of the ways that participants use shortcuts when
answering survey questions, such as response sets or fence-sitting?
Explain and give examples. | back 21 - Participants can use shortcuts by…
- weakens construct
validity b/c respondents are not saying what they really think
- ex. answering a long survey with all positively
agree
- ex. people may answer in the middle (or
say “I don’t know”) when the question is confusing or unclear
- weakens construct validity b/c it suggests that some
responders don’t have an opinion when they actually do
- response sets (nondifferentiation): shortcuts
respondents may use to answer items in a long survey, rather than
responding to the content of each item
- fence-sitting:
playing it safe by answering in the middle of the scale for every
question in a survey or interview
|
front 22
What are some techniques that researchers can use to avoid
these shortcuts? | back 22 - To avoid these, researchers can
- response sets:
include reverse-worded items, change items to mean the
opposite
- helps slow people down to actually answer the
question more carefully
- more construct validity b/c
high or low averages would be measuring true happiness or
unhappiness rather than reluctant answering
- fence-sitting:
- researchers can take away the
neural option
- drawback → when people really don’t
have an answer, choosing a side is an invalid representation
of their true neural stance
- use force-choice
questions where participants must pick one of two answers
- drawback → can frustrate people who feel their opinion
is between the 2 answers
|
front 23
In addition to shortcuts, what are three other problems that
can occur on surveys, and how can they be avoided? | back 23 - Socially desirable responding (faking good): giving answers on
a survey that make one look better that make than one really
does
- faking bad: less common, but is the opposite
- can be avoided → ensure participants that their responses are
anonymous and remind them before sensitive questions, ask people’s
friends to rate them, use computerized measures to evaluate people’s
opinions
- can be inaccurate b/c when asked people to describe why
they are thinking, feeling, and behaving the way they do, people
would give inaccurate responses
- memories for significant life experiences can be
accurate
- ex. adverse childhood experiences
(ACEs)
- people’s certainty about their memories
might not match their accuracy
- vividness and confidence
are unrelated to how accurate memories are
- trying to look good
- self-reporting “more
than they can know”
- Self-reporting memories or
events
|
front 24
response sets (nondifferentiation) | back 24 - shortcuts respondents may use to answer items in a long survey,
rather than responding to the content of each item
- weakens construct validity b/c respondents are not saying what
they really think
- ex. answering a long survey with all
positively agree
|
| back 25 - playing it safe by answering in the middle of the scale for
every question in a survey or interview
- ex. people may
answer in the middle (or say “I don’t know”) when the question is
confusing or unclear
- weakens construct validity b/c it
suggests that some responders don’t have an opinion when they
actually do
|
| back 26 - Socially desirable responding (faking good): giving answers on
a survey that make one look better that make than one really
does
- faking bad: less common, but is the opposite
- can be avoided → ensure participants that their responses are
anonymous and remind them before sensitive questions, ask people’s
friends to rate them, use computerized measures to evaluate people’s
opinions
|
front 27
self-reporting “more than they can know” | back 27 can be inaccurate b/c when asked people to describe why they are
thinking, feeling, and behaving the way they do, people would give
inaccurate responses |
front 28
Self-reporting memories or events | back 28 - memories for significant life experiences can be accurate
- ex. adverse childhood experiences (ACEs)
- people’s certainty about their memories might not match their
accuracy
- vividness and confidence are unrelated to how
accurate memories are
|
front 29
What kind of claim is best made with observational data? Why?
When and how can observations be better than self-reports? When are
they worse? | back 29 - Frequency claims can be best made with observational data b/c
they can use it for observational variables. Can also work to
operationalize variables in association claims and causal claims.
Need to depend on if the observational measures have good construct
validity
- ex. watching families eat dinner
- Observations can be better than self-reports because they can
tell a more accurate story.
- ex. if researchers ask
participants to estimate how many words they spoke in a day, that
would be difficult to do and wouldn’t give an accurate
measure.
- Observations can be worse when the construct
validity is threatened, this being when observer bias, effects, and
reactivity are present.
|
front 30
Explain some of the pitfalls (e.g., observer bias, observer
effects, reactivity) when making observations. | back 30 - Observer bias: when observers see (confirmation bias) what they
expect to see
- A bias occurs when observer expectations
influence the interpretations of participant behaviors or the
outcomes of the study
- Observer effects: when
participants confirm observer expectation
- a change in
the behavior of participants in the direction of observer
expectations (expectancy effect)
- ex. Clever Hans = horse
that can do the math, but was caused by the trainer presenting
subtle non-verbal cues
- reactivity
- a change
in behavior of study participants (such as acting less
spontaneously) because they are aware they are being watched
|
front 31
What can be done to remedy these? | back 31 - Can prevent observer effects and bias by
- training
observers well by using clear rating instructions (codebooks)
- use multiple observers → can access interrater reliability of
their measures
- Can prevent reactivity by
- blending in or making unobtrusive observations
- wait it
out measure the behavior results - measure traces of a particular
behavior left behind
|
front 32
Explain why external validity matters for frequency claims. | back 32 - When interrogating external validity, we ask whether the
results of a particular study can be generalized to some larger
population of interest
- Important because external validity
concerns both samples and settings
- sample; a
researcher would intend the results to generalize other
populations
- settings; researcher would intend the results
to generalize other settings
|
front 33
What is the difference between a population, a sample, and a census? | back 33 - population: a larger group from which a sample is drawn; the
group to which the study’s conclusions are intended to be applied
(the entire set of people or products interested in)
- ex. when eating a bag of chips, the box/bag is from the
population
- sample: a group of people, animals, or
cases used in a study; a subset of the population of interest
(smaller set taken from the population)
- ex. the single
bite of a chip from the bag of chips
- census: a set
of observations that contains all members of the population of
interest
- ex. tasting every chip in the bag
(population)
|
| back 34 - a larger group from which a sample is drawn; the group to which
the study’s conclusions are intended to be applied (the entire set
of people or products interested in)
- ex. when eating a
bag of chips, the box/bag is from the population
|
| back 35 - a group of people, animals, or cases used in a study; a subset
of the population of interest (smaller set taken from the
population)
- ex. the single bite of a chip from the bag
of chips
|
| back 36 - a set of observations that contains all members of the
population of interest
- ex. tasting every chip in the
bag (population)
|
front 37
Explain several probability sampling techniques and give examples. | back 37 - Simple random sampling
- systematic sampling
- cluster sampling
- multi-stage sampling:
- stratified random sampling
- oversampling
- random
assignment
|
front 38
sampling those who volunteer | back 38 - self-selection: a form of sampling bias that occurs when a
sample only contains people who volunteer to participate
- ex. this can be someone rating an item online, hard to speculate
if they are actually rating it or not
|
front 39
sampling only those who are easy to contact | back 39 - convenience sampling: choosing a sample on those who are easit
to access and readily available; a biased sampling technique
- ex. psychology studies conducted by psychology professors would
use using college students as participants. This is easy to reach
sample and may not represent other populations who represent those
less educated, older or younger
|
front 40
Explain several ways of obtaining a sample that might result
in a biased sample. | back 40 - sampling those who are easy to contact
- sampling those
who volunteer
|
front 41
Be sure to know the difference between random sampling and
random assignment, and when each is necessary. | back 41 - random sampling: when researchers create a sample using some
random method and each number of the population has an equal chance
of being in the sample
- Necessary: enhances internal
validity
- random assignment: when a random method is
being used to put participants in separate groups
- Necessary: enhances external validity
|
front 42
Explain why a representative sample is not always necessary. | back 42 no necessary when external validity is not vital in the study |
| back 43 - the most basic form of probability sampling, in which the
sample is chosen completely at random from the population of
interest
- ex. when pollsters need a random sample, they
program computers to randomly select phone numbers or home
addresses from a database of eligible people
|
| back 44 - in which a researcher uses a randomly chosen number N, and
counts off every Nth member of a population to ac achieve a
sample
- ex. If the population of interest is a room full
of students, the researcher would start with the fourth person in
the room and then count off, choosing every 7th person until the
sample is the desired size
|
| back 45 - probability sampling technique where clusters of participants
within the population of interest are selected at random
- followed by data collection from all individuals in each
cluster
|
| back 46 - a variation of stratified random sampling in which the
researcher intentionally overrepresent one or more groups
- ex. when one would have a large sample from one group when it
is actually a small percentage representation
|
| back 47 - assigning participants into different experimental groups, only
used in experimental designs
- ex. in an experiment
testing how exercise affects well-being, random assignment would
make it likely that the people in the treatment and comparison
groups are about equally happy at the start.
|
front 48
stratified random sampling: | back 48 - researcher identifies particular demographic categories, or
strata, and then randomly selects individuals within each
category
- ex. sampling a population of 1,000 Canadians
that includes South Asian descent as the same portion as the
Canadian population. These are already 2 categories. One would
have to have 2 groups from them but randomly selected.
|
| back 49 - (similar to a cluster) involving at least 2 stages: a random
sample of clusters followed by a random sample of clusters followed
by a random sample of people within the selected cluster
- ex. a researcher would start with a list of high schools in
the state and select a random sample of students from each of the
selected schools
|
| back 50 - when researchers create a sample using some random method and
each number of the population has an equal chance of being in the
sample
- Necessary: enhances internal validity
|
| back 51 - when a random method is being used to put participants in
separate groups
- Necessary: enhances external
validity
|
front 52
Explain what types of studies support association claims | back 52 - Bivariate correlation: an association that involves exactly 2
variables
- 3 types → positive, negative, and zero
- Use studies where you measure the first variable and the
second variable in the same group of people
- you then use
graphs and statistics to describe the type of relationship they
(variables) have with each other
|
front 53
Explain construct validity of an association claim | back 53 - Ask about the construct validity of each variable
- How
well was each of the two variables measured?
- You would need
to ask questions about researchers’ operationalizations of the
variables
- questions one would ask after knowing the kinds of
measurements
- Does the measure have good reliability? Is
it measuring what it’s intended to measure? What is the evidence
for its face validity, its concurrent validity, its discriminant
and convergent validity?
|
front 54
Explain each of the six questions that need to be answered
when checking statistical validity for an association claim. | back 54 - 1) How strong is the relationship?
- all
associations are not equal; some are stronger than others
- How precise is the estimate?
A study’s correlation
coefficient is the point estimate of the true correlation in the
population - 3) Has it been replicated?
Can conduct the
study again (replication) and find multiple estimates - 4)
Could outliers be affecting the association? As a small sample has a
wider CI. outliers matter the most when a sample is small
- 5) Is there a restriction of range?
- restriction
of range: in a bivariate correlation, the absence of a full range
of possible scores on one of the variables, so the relationship
from the sample underestimates the true correlation
- 6) Is the association Curlinear?
an association between 2
variables which is not a straight line; as one variable increases,
the level of the other variable increases and then decreases |
front 55
Explain why internal validity is not possible with association
claims. | back 55 - there must be no plausible alternative explanations for the
relationship between the 2 variables
- the potential third
variable can be a problem because of
- spurious
association: bivariate association that is attributable only to
systematic mean differences in subgroups within the sample; the
original associations are not present within the subgroups
- when proposing a third variable, it is not necessary to
present an internal validity problem
- when
interrogating a simple association claim, it is not necessary to
focus on interval validity as long as it’s just that
|
front 56
Explain external validity for association claims, including
what a moderating variable is/does. | back 56 - when interrogating external validity, recall that the size of
the sample does not matter as much as the way the sample
was selected from the population of interest
- Importance of
external validity → shows when the pattern of results is the same
from both groups no matter if numbers are the same
- moderating variables
- moderator: a variable that
depending on its level, changes the relationship between 2 other
variables
- moderators can inform external validity
|
front 57
what a moderating variable is/does | back 57 - moderating variables
- moderator: a variable that
depending on its level, changes the relationship between 2 other
variables
- moderators can inform external validity
|
front 58
How do longitudinal studies help establish causation? There
are three types of correlations that can be tested with longitudinal
studies. Explain and give examples of each. | back 58 - Longitudinal studies can help establish causation by providing
evidence for temporal precedence and be adapted to test causal
claims
-
Cross-sectional
correlations
- a correlation between 2
variables that are measured at the same time
- Shows
covariance
-
Autocorrelations
- the correlation of one variable with itself, measured
at two different times
- this shows stability between the
variables
-
Cross-lag
Correlations
a correlation between an easier measure of one variable and a
later measure of another variable addresses the directionality
and helps establish temporal precedence |
front 59
Cross-sectional correlations | back 59 - a correlation between 2 variables that are measured at the same
time
- Shows covariance
|
| back 60 - the correlation of one variable with itself, measured at two
different times
- this shows stability between the
variables
|
| back 61 - a correlation between an easier measure of one variable and a
later measure of another variable
- addresses the
directionality and helps establish temporal precedence
|
front 62
Explain why we cannot always do experiments to establish
causation in social science research. | back 62 - in many cases, participants cannot be randomly assigned to a
causal variable
- people cannot be assigned to
preferences
- people can’t change their parenting styles
- maybe unethical to assign participants - should not!
- unethical to assign some people (children) to certain
conditions
|
front 63
How do multiple-regression analyses help address the question
of internal validity? | back 63 - multiple regression
- helps rule out some third
variable, but not a foolproof way of doing so
- computes
the relationship between a predictor variable and criterion
variable, controlling for other predictor variables
|
front 64
Explain what a mediating variable is/does, and compare it to a
moderator variable and the third variable problem. | back 64 - Mediating variable: ask why/how, for relationships that always
exist, why it is related (meaningful variable)
- internal to the causal variable (not problematic)
- Moderator variable: when and for whom the relationship
exists
- 3rd variable: external and not part of the
explanation
- external to the 2 variables in the bivariate
correlation (problematic)
|
| back 65 - ask why/how, for relationships that always exist, why it is
related (meaningful variable)
- internal to the causal
variable (not problematic)
|
| back 66 - when and for whom the relationship exists
- can change
the relationship between the other two variables (making it more
intense or less intense).
|
| back 67 - external and not part of the explanation
- external to
the 2 variables in the bivariate correlation (problematic)
|
front 68
Explain how we might use statistics to control for third
variables. Why would we want to do this? | back 68 - evaluate whether a relationship between two variables still
holds when they control for another variable
- “control
for” - holding a 3rd variable at a constant level when
investigating the association between 2 other variables
- can mean to recognize that testing a third variable with
multiple regression means identifying subgroups
|
front 69
Explain what “beta” is in regression analyses. | back 69 - one beta for each predictor variable
- beta is similar
to r but does more
- both have a negative and positive
beta
- positive - indicates a positive relationship
between the predictor variable and the criterion variable, when
the other predictor variables are statically controlled for
- negative - indicates a negative relationship between two
variables, (when other variables are controlled for)
- when beta is at zero, no relationship
- higher beta →
stronger relationship
- The smaller the beta → weaker the
relationship
|